Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Detection distances in desert dwelling, high duty cycle echolocators: A test of the foraging habitat hypothesis

  • Nikita M. Finger,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Current address: Department of Biological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, United States of America

    Affiliation Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa

  • Marc Holderied,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

  • David S. Jacobs

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    david.jacobs@uct.ac.za

    Affiliation Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa

Abstract

High Duty Cycle (HDC) echolocating bats use high frequency echolocation pulses that are clutter resistant, but their high frequencies give them limited range. Despite their unique ability to reject background clutter while simultaneously detecting fluttering prey, the frequency of their echolocation pulses has a strong correlation with level of environmental clutter, lower frequency pulses of HDC bats being associated with more open environments. The Foraging Habitat Hypothesis (FHH) proposes that the ecological significance of these lower frequency pulses in HDC bats in open environments is that they allow longer prey detection distances. To test the FHH, we compared the frequencies, Source Levels (SLs) and detection distances of Rhinolophus capensis, a HDC bat that has been shown to vary its call frequency in relation to habitat structure. As a further test of the FHH we investigated the SLs and detection distances of Rhinolophus damarensis (a heterospecific species that occurs in the same open desert environment as R. capensis but echolocates at a higher dominant pulse frequency). In the open desert, R. capensis emitted both lower frequency and higher SL pulses giving them longer detection distances than R. capensis in the cluttered fynbos. SL contributed more to differences in detection distances in both R. capensis and R. damarensis than frequency. In a few instances, R. damarensis achieved similar detection distances to desert–inhabiting R. capensis by emitting much higher SLs despite their average SLs being lower. These results suggest that lower frequency echolocation pulses are not a prerequisite for open desert living but may increase detection distance while avoiding energetic costs required for high SLs.

Introduction

The success of bats as nocturnal hunters can be attributed to their use of echolocation [1,2]. The acoustic characteristics of their biosonar pulses play a crucial role in how bats detect, localize, and classify objects [38]. The distance over which bats can detect prey is determined by two key echolocation properties: pulse frequency and source level (SL) [9]. External properties of the environment, such as climate and habitat structure correlate with variation in pulse frequency [1014]. One of the ways in which habitat structure influences echolocating bats is through clutter, the number of echoes produced by objects, e.g. vegetation, other than those from the target of interest. Variation in pulse frequency in responses to different levels of clutter has been observed in bats that use a clutter rejection echolocation system, i.e. High Duty Cycle (HDC) bats, despite their unique ability to reject background clutter and simultaneously detect fluttering prey [1517]. For example, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum occupy a variety of different habitats with various levels of clutter/vegetation. In relatively open habitats this species emits lower echolocation frequencies [18].

The level of atmospheric attenuation a bat is subjected to in its habitat may impact the source level at which a bat emits its echolocation pulse. Atmospheric attenuation is a function of the frequency of the pulse; higher frequency pulses are attenuated more strongly [11,12,19]. An intricate interaction between pulse frequency, atmospheric attenuation, and the source levels at which bats emit their echolocation pulses could exist. Climate affects atmospheric attenuation, and hence echolocation, by a complex interaction between temperature and humidity amongst other climatic variables [11,12,19,20]. The interaction between these variables (i.e. climate, atmospheric attenuation, and a bats echolocation pulse parameters) can vary from arid to mesic habitats. Furthermore, differences in temperature and rainfall between arid and mesic habitats, are likely to be reflected in more open (less clutter) and denser vegetation (more clutter), respectively [12,20]. Bats, even those using clutter-rejecting HDC echolocation, foraging in low-clutter habitats would likely benefit from longer detection distances. Increased detection distances would allow them to search greater volumes of space more efficiently. The interaction between pulse frequency, atmospheric attenuation, source levels and clutter, is therefore crucial to understanding the ecological significance of differences in frequency and their resultant impact on detection distances in HDC bats.

The influence of habitat structure was formalized in the Foraging Habitat Hypothesis (FHH). The FHH proposes a close relationship between the range of echolocation frequencies used by bats and the degree of clutter they are exposed to in different habitats [10]. The FHH was first proposed for Low Duty Cycle (LDC) bats. In LDC bats the FHH proposes that bats increase pulse frequency in obstacle rich habitats for increased resolution of prey against background vegetation [10,2123]. The ecological significance of the FHH in clutter rejection foragers (i.e. HDC bats) is less well established despite marked variation in echolocation pulse frequencies across biomes [18,24]. Unlike LDC bats, HDC bats have the unique ability to reject background clutter and simultaneously detect fluttering prey using high frequencies, Doppler Shift Compensation, and the acoustic glints generated from the wing beats of insects [1517]. As a result, they can easily distinguish flying prey from background vegetation. Furthermore, as clutter specialists many of the smaller HDC bats species evolved the use of very high echolocation pulse frequencies allowing them to retrieve echoes from small objects (leaves, twigs, wings of insects) [25]. HDC bats place most of their acoustic energy into the second harmonic which is at a higher frequency than the fundamental [26]. At such high frequencies increases in resolution would be minimal because differences in frequency are not large enough to produce substantial differences in target strength [27]. Unlike LDC bats, observed differences in frequencies in HDC bats in environments characterized by different degrees of clutter cannot, therefore, be attributed to an increase in prey resolution in obstacle rich environments. The use of high frequencies subjects the echolocation pulses of these small HDC bats to extreme atmospheric attenuation resulting in shorter prey detection distances. In open environments these small bats would be unable to increase their flight speed to search the open space at a faster rate. This would potentially be disadvantageous because their high frequency pulses would cover a smaller volume of space. Despite this disadvantage, small HDC species occupy open environments such as deserts. However, they tend to use echolocation pulses of lower frequencies [18,24]. Such lower pulse frequencies may be associated with lower levels of clutter. If so, under the FHH modified for HDC bats, we propose that the lower frequency pulses used by HDC bats in open environments result in longer prey detection distances allowing them to search open space more efficiently. If so, differences in echolocation frequency between HDC bats foraging in open habitats and those foraging in more cluttered habitats should result in longer detection distances in the open habitats.

Detection distance is strongly affected by both frequency and SL [9]. Increasing pulse SL increases the distance over which that pulse can return echo information loud enough to be heard by the echolocating bat (i.e. the operational distance of echolocation) [9]. Technological advances in microphone array analysis have revealed that LDC bats, regardless of size, use extremely intense (high SL) echolocation pulses in the field [2831]. However, the ecological significance of SL has only been explored in a few studies on LDC bats and these have shown its functional impact on prey detection distance. For example, Surlykke and Kalko [30] found that LDC bat species in the local tropical assemblage compensated for frequency-dependent attenuation by using higher SLs to achieve comparable detection distances. In contrast, two species in a temperate region with different foraging strategies and habitats (open air versus clutter) used a combination of frequency and SL to achieve appropriate but different detection distances in their respective habitats [32]. These findings suggest that both the physical properties of the habitat (i.e. vegetation structure, climate) and the specific ecology of the focal species must be considered when testing the FHH. Currently, the only information on SL and detection distances in HDC bats comes from laboratory studies [3335]. Recent advances in cross-correlation methods (microphone array analysis) use the FM component of HDC bat pulses to accurately determine differences in arrival times of the pulses at each microphone in the array and therefore the position of the bat relative to the microphones. Using this method SLs of HDC bats can be calculated from the field and lab. Field studies are vital to understanding the natural SLs and resulting detection distances emitted by bats in their respective environments.

Our study tested the modified version of the FHH in two HDC species; Rhinolophus capensis, which occupies a range of arid and mesic habitats, and R. damarensis which occupies mainly arid habitats and is sympatric with an R. capensis population in the desert. R. capensis successfully inhabits a variety of biomes characterised by different degrees of clutter from fynbos (an evergreen, Mediterranean type shrubland that occurs in the eastern and western cape provinces of South Africa) to desert [24]. Odendaal et al. [24] found a positive correlation between level of clutter and frequency in these populations, with lower frequencies occurring at lower levels of clutter. Our study investigated two populations of R. capensis: R. capensis in the desert (echolocation frequency: 75 kHz) and R. capensis in the fynbos (echolocation frequency: 84 kHz) [24]. The desert population of R. capensis deviated from the correlation between pulse frequency and body size more than any of the other populations i.e. it had a lower pulse frequency than predicted by its body size [24]. Similarly, it also deviated more than the other populations from the relationship between pulse frequency and clutter [24]

Rhinolophus damarensis, inhabits the same desert as R. capensis and other less arid habitats, but uses an echolocation pulse frequency equivalent to R. capensis inhabiting the fynbos. The use of higher pulse frequencies in the desert should result in much shorter detection distances for R. damarensis than R. capensis. Given its similar size and wing loading to R. capensis [36], this would place R. damarensis at a considerable disadvantage because the volume of space within which they searched for prey could be greatly reduced in comparison. In addition to frequency, SL also influences detection distance [9,37,38]. For R. damarensis to achieve comparable detection distances to R. capensis in the desert they would need to use higher SLs (as was found for LDC bats) [30].

The FHH hypothesis proposes that differences between desert and fynbos R. capensis are the result of ecological selection for longer detection distances in desert bats in response to lower levels of clutter (increased volumes of space) in the desert [10]. If so, 1) detection distances for desert bats i.e. R. capensis and R. damarensis should be similar to each other but longer than fynbos R. capensis 2) SLs of R. damarensis should be higher than desert R. capensis if they are to have similar detection distances i.e. similar responses to open space given the differences in their pulse frequencies 3) differences in detection distances in R. capensis should largely be the result of differences in pulse frequency.

Methods

Study species

Our focal species is the endemic cape horseshoe bat, Rhinolophus capensis. R. capensis, is a medium sized bat weighing around 10 g– 16 g with a forearm length of 47 mm– 52 mm [27]. R. capensis forages in or near cluttered habitats [27,39]. Their diet consists mainly of Lepidoptera and Coleoptera [27]. R. capensis is largely restricted to the narrow coastal belt of the western and southern coast of South Africa between the coastline and the great escarpment encompassing a variety of different biomes [24]. Body size and echolocation frequency (up to 10 kHz gradual change across its range) vary across the geographic distribution of R. capensis [24,40]. Spatially defined mitochondrial groups do exist but there is gene flow across its range [24]. In addition, mitochondrial DNA structure revealed minimal genetic structure among different populations of R. capensis in Southern Africa indicating that the desert and fynbos populations are indeed the same species [24]. A study that used nuclear introns as markers for phylogenetic reconstruction also confirmed this [41]. R. capensis range is characterised by relatively open habitats in the North and West (desert and karoo biomes) spanning habitats characterised by an increase in clutter in the East (fynbos, albany thicket and forest) [24,42]. These differences in vegetation are the result of a latitudinal aridity gradient, aridity increasing northwards and longitudinal seasonality shift in rainfall [43,44]. South Africa is predominantly a summer rainfall region (with approximately half of R. capensis range occurring in these regions) with a seasonal shift in rainfall that is caused by the winter rainfall zone that occurs along the southwestern and southern tip of the African continent. The rainfall in the winter rainfall zone is predominantly caused by cold fronts from polar cyclone systems originating over the South Atlantic. The diminishing influence of these polar frontal systems in the north is correlated with a decrease in rainfall and an increase in aridity [43,44].

Despite gene flow in R. capensis, the marked differences in rainfall and vegetation across their range has resulted in a stable relationship between pulse frequency and increasing vegetation; pulse frequency increases from open areas in the Northwest (75 kHz) to highly cluttered habitats in the Southeast (86 kHz) [24]. Differences in body mass do not explain differences in resting frequency because populations with similar body mass have different echolocation frequencies. Similarly, relative humidity does not explain these substantial differences in pulse frequency [24].

In the north of their range, R. capensis co-occurs with Rhinolophus damarensis and in the south with Rhinolophus clivosus [24]. R. damarensis (forearm length: 47 mm—52 mm) and R. clivosus (forearm length: 56 mm– 57 mm) are both medium sized bats [27]. R. capensis echolocates at approximately 9 kHz lower than their respective heterospecifics in the northern and southern regions where they overlap with these two species. Variation in pulse frequency, body size, and the environment of R. capensis, coupled with the occurrence of sympatric congenerics in parts of their range provide an excellent opportunity for investigating factors that influence intraspecific phenotypic divergence.

Sampling sites

Bats were recorded in two biomes with contrasting clutter levels (desert and fynbos) over the summer months (Desert: January–February 2017, Fynbos: March 2017). Levels of clutter can be estimated by satellite-based vegetation assessment using the metric Normalized Difference Vegetation Index (NDVI). The NDVI ranges from -1 to 1 with negative values corresponding to little or no vegetation cover and positive values corresponding to different degrees of vegetation cover [45]. The desert biome sampled in this study has a NDVI index of 0.22 and the fynbos has a NDVI index of 0.45 [24]. We recorded bats both close to day roosts (mix of commuting and foraging activity) and in foraging habitats. In the desert, R. capensis was recorded during emergence at Wondergat cave (a day roost) and at a nearby foraging area (dried riverbed) shortly after emergence. Foraging sites were identified by the presence of feeding buzzes in on-site recordings of bat echolocation pulses. The desert foraging site was located 3.9 km away from Wondergat cave (28°25’S 16°53’E) in the Lekkersing area. R. damarensis was recorded in the desert biome during emergence from Numeesberg mineshaft (28°26’S 17°01’E) and at the Orange River cave (28°42’S 17°32’E). Orange River cave was considered both a cave and foraging site because of the presence of feeding buzzes in the echolocation recordings of bats in this area. In the south coast, R. capensis was recorded at Hot Hole cave (34°27’S 20°26’E) in De Hoop Nature Reserve. Hot Hole cave was characterised as both cave and foraging site. Echolocation pulses recorded at Hot Hole cave were therefore grouped into an emergence/foraging category because they could not be separated.

Calculating source levels (predictions 1–3)

Calculation of the source levels (SLs) (sound pressure level of the bats echolocation pulses at a predefined distance of 10 cm in front of the bat in the current study, in decibels [dB]) required that the position of the bat in relation to each microphone be known. Microphone array analysis calculates SL by reconstructing bats flight path in three dimensions in relation to each array [4648] (Fig 1). Flight paths were re-constructed in Matlab (Mathworks, Version 2013, Natick, United States) following a script used in Holderied and von Helversen [48]. Individual flight paths were only constructed from search phase sequences, even when feeding buzzes were present, i.e. in recordings from foraging areas feeding buzzes were not included in the analysis. Feeding buzzes were distinguished as a sequence of pulses towards the end of a train of pulses where the bat increased its duty cycle to around 90% [49]. We also analysed passes where only one individual was recorded flying in the area covered by the microphone array. From a processing perspective this facilitated trajectory generation by avoiding call-assignation ambiguities, and experimentally this minimized potential behavioural bias created by the presence of nearby conspecifics.

thumbnail
Fig 1. An example of a three-dimensional flight path acquired by acoustic tracking.

The illustrated flight path was reconstructed from the echolocation pulse sequences of Rhinolophus capensis as it emerged from Wondergat Cave in the desert biome. Circles indicate echolocation pulses and arrows indicate the bat’s direction of travel towards the microphones. The Image was edited in PhotoScape (MOOII Tech, version 3.7, Korea).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268138.g001

Acoustic flight path reconstructions were based on the differences in the Times of Arrival (TOA) of each pulse at each of the microphones as the bats flew towards the arrays [48,50,51]. TOA differences were calculated using cross-correlation analysis, which measures the similarity between two signals as a function of their relative time delay. Certain pulse designs produce clearer i.e. narrower and less error-prone cross-correlation results [52]. Short broadband FM pulses, with steep frequency modulation are best suited for cross-correlation techniques in contrast to constant frequency signals, that preclude unambiguous measurement of differences in arrival times at the microphones in the array [5255]. The TOA of the pulses at each of the eight microphones was therefore based on the terminal frequency-modulated component of the echolocation pulses of R. capensis. The voltage generated by each pulse at the centre microphones of two arrays were converted to recorded dB SPL (taking propagation losses into account) and then converted back to SLs calculated at 10 cm from the bat (calculated for each pulse in a flight path) (Matlab script, ©Holderied). SLs were corrected for both atmospheric attenuation (absorption losses that were dependent on temperature, humidity, and atmospheric pressure) and spreading losses (a decrease in sound pressure levels as a sound propagates away from a source) [9,56,57] based on the distance of the bat to the microphone and the recorded frequency of each pulse [48]. It is important to note that beam width of the emitted call also affects sonar distance by focusing the available energy in one direction or radiating it in many [58]. Techniques to study beam width in HDC bats are new and yet to be applied in field studies. Our field study was also designed to measure accurate SLs of HDC bats rather than measuring their beam width. We minimized the potential bias resulting from off-axis recordings in the following ways. In linear flight bats are likely to point the beam towards their direction of travel [59]. So we chose straight sections of the bat’s flight corridor with bats flying towards the recording microphones. We then only analysed the highest calculated SL per trajectory, thereby selecting the call most closely directed at the recording microphone. Furthermore, the high directionality of HDC bat sound emissions means that only calls directed at the arrays will have sufficient acoustic energy for tracking at all microphones, which already drastically reduces the amount of off- axis recordings in our dataset. Dynamic range was calculated for R. capensis in the different recording areas by subtracting maximum SLs minus minimum SLs (species level: across all the acoustic flight paths recorded). Automatic parameter measurements (Avisoft-SASLab Pro, v5.2, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Glienicke, Germany) were used to measure echolocation pulse frequency.

Prevailing atmospheric conditions (temperature, humidity, air pressure, and wind speed) at the time of recording of each echolocation pulse, for the determination of bat flight paths, were recorded to allow precise calculation of SLs. Atmospheric variables were recorded using a weather station (Wireless Pro Weather Station, Oregon Scientific, Oregon, USA). The weather station was set up within a few metres of the arrays and three metres above the ground, the maximum flight height of R. capensis [27].

Calculating detection distances (testing prediction 1)

Maximum detection distances for detecting different sized prey were calculated using the following formula: DT = SL+ TLA + TLS + TS [48,6062] where DT is the detection threshold; TLA is transmission loss due to absorption; TLS is the transmission loss due to spherical spreading and TS is the target strength. TLA and TLS are functions of distance. TLA was calculated using the National Physical Laboratory online calculator ((http://resource.npl.co.uk/acoustics/techguides/absorption/). The calculator uses echolocation pulse frequency (frequency of the pulse with the greatest SL in each pass), local atmospheric conditions (temperature, humidity, and atmospheric pressure), and target type to calculate TLA for each pass. Detection distances were calculated using maximum SL (dB SPL) for each pass and the corresponding frequency of that echolocation pulse. The auditory threshold (detection threshold) of the bat was assumed to be 20 dB SPL for bats flying under natural conditions [9]. Target strength is the acoustic energy reflected from an ensonified echolocation signal on a target.

Detection distances were calculated for different prey sizes because target strength varies with the size of the object that reflects the impinging pulse and generates the echo. Each size class has its own target strength, which is used to determine the effective distance at which bats detect different sized prey. Target strengths for each category of insects was assigned as follows: small (-65 dB), medium (-50 dB), and large (-40 dB) in accordance with Stilz and Schnitzler [9]. These target strengths were based on size ranges (4 mm—28 mm) of insects given in previous studies for R. capensis [48,63]. To determine if the size range of prey available to bats in each habitat (and therefore the target strength) matched those given by these studies, two insect light traps were set up at each recording site at night (from dusk till dawn) over the same time frame in which the acoustic recordings were made. Ultraviolet black lights (Sylvania Blacklight FC22W/350BL UV lamp, Massachusetts, United States) and medium sized buckets (volume: 17.68 L) were used. Collected insect samples were stored in 70% ethyl alcohol and kept in a freezer until analysis. Sample specimens from the orders Coleoptera (beetles) and Lepidoptera (moths), common in the diet of R. capensis in South Africa, were analysed [27]. Body lengths of these insects were used as a measure of size [27]. Measurements of body lengths were taken from the hind-most tip of the abdomen to the forward most part of the head (excluding the antennae). Sizes of smaller insect were measured using a stereo microscope (Leica EZ4 model, St. Gallen, Switzerland) fitted with a calibrated eyepiece graticule [64,65]. Sizes of larger insects were measured using handheld dial calipers with a ± 0.02 mm increment. The range of sizes of insects collected in this study was then compared to those used by Stilz and Schnitzler [9] to calculate different target strengths.

Calculating contribution of frequency and SL to detection distance (testing prediction 3)

To determine whether pulse frequencies or SLs has the greatest influence on prey detection distances of R. capensis in each habitat and in which of these habitats the effect was larger (Prediction 3) we compared detection distances calculated using maximum and minimum values for each parameter i.e. SL and frequency. Detection distance was calculated for each acoustic flight path using the atmospheric conditions present at the time of the pass and the maximum and minimum values recorded for that night for one parameter while keeping the second parameter constant (i.e. using the average recorded value for the second parameter for that night). These detection distances were calculated across one night of recording in each habitat. Within the desert biome, the acoustic parameter that had a greater effect on detection distances (frequency or SL) was determined by comparing detection distances calculated using atmospheric conditions of flight paths recorded at Wondergat cave using a) average frequency of desert passes and varied SLs (average SLs of desert bats versus average SLs of fynbos bats) b) the average SLs of desert bats and different frequencies (average frequency of desert bats versus average frequency of fynbos bats) c) the average frequency and SL for each biome (desert and fynbos).

Clutter index

A laser range finder (Leica Disto S910, Leica, St. Gallen, Switzerland) measured the distance of objects relative to a reference point creating a three dimensional (3D) image of the recording environment. The 3D image was then used to compare the degree of clutter in each recording environment by creating a clutter index (the percent of space occupied by vegetation or objects). To create this index a grid (8 m width × 8 m length × 3 m height) was constructed over the recording area using thin cord. Grid size was determined by the recording area (the maximum area over which flight paths could be constructed) as well as the average maximum height at which R. capensis flies when foraging [27]. A measurement point was counted for every metre on the 8 m × 8 m grid if vegetation or an object (e.g. fence, rock) was present. For every metre, measurement points were taken at 0.25 m intervals (starting 0.25 m off the ground) up to 3 m high. Percent clutter (i.e. the clutter index) was calculated by dividing the points recorded by the total possible points for that recording area.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses of acoustic data were performed using STATISTICA (StatSoft, Inc., Version 12, Tulsa, USA) with a global level of significance of 5%. Data were log transformed (= log (#, 10)) because of the different scales of measurement of the parameters. Predictions 1 and 2 were tested using Generalised Linear Models (GLMs). GLMs compared response variables (frequencies, SLs, and detection distances) to categorical variables (species, biomes, and recording environments (foraging versus emergence). Each flight path was an independent variable that consisted of values for its acoustic parameters. Detection distances were calculated using maximum SL (dB SPL) for each pass and the corresponding frequency of that echolocation pulse. A GLM and Wilcoxon Matched Pairs were used to test Prediction 3. A GLM compared the detection indices for each biome. The detection index for each biome was calculated by subtracting detection distances using maximum acoustic parameter values minus detection distance using minimum acoustic parameter values. In the GLM the detection index was the response variable and the two biomes the categorical variable. Wilcoxon Matched Pairs Tests compared detection distance using maximum values for each acoustic parameter against detection distances using minimum values for each acoustic parameter within each biome. Within the desert biome, Wilcoxon Matched Pairs Tests tested for differences in detection distances calculated using the average acoustic parameters of either the desert or fynbos biome.

Ethical statement

Our study followed the guidelines set out in the American Society of Mammalogists for recording, capturing, and handling bats [66,67]. These methods were approved by the University of Cape Town’s Faculty of Science Animal Ethics Committee (2016/v1/DJ) and Biological Safety Committee. Permits were attained for research in both the Eastern Cape (Cape Nature: AAA007-00012-0052) and Northern Cape (FAUNA 0013/2016) where the study occurred.

Results

In the desert biome, 21 flight paths were constructed for R. capensis during emergence from Wondergat and 19 flight paths were constructed from the desert foraging site. For R. damarensis, 13 and 19 flight paths were constructed for emergence from Numeesberg mineshaft and the Orange River cave, respectively. In the fynbos biome a combined total of 28 flight paths for R. capensis were constructed during emergence from Hot Hole cave and whilst the bats were foraging in the area around Hot Hole cave. Prey sampled in both desert and fynbos biomes ranged in size from 4 mm—28 mm (Table 1) and therefore fell into the same size ranges of insect prey reported in previous studies [48,63]. Recording sites ranged in level of clutter (Table 2 and Fig 2). In areas where R. capensis was recorded, fynbos had the highest degree of clutter (40% clutter) followed by the desert foraging site (24% clutter) and Wondergat cave (7% clutter). In areas where R. damarensis was recorded, Orange River cave (19% clutter) had the most clutter followed by Numeesberg mineshaft (13% clutter).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Photographic images taken of recording sites in South Africa of R. capensis and R. damarensis.

Images are listed from lowest to highest clutter a) Wondergat cave (desert), b) Numeesberg Mineshaft (desert), c) Orange River cave (desert), d) R. capensis foraging site (desert), e) Hot Hole cave (fynbos).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268138.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Morphometric data from collected insect samples.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268138.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Acoustic parameters (average ± SD) of echolocation pulses from R. capensis and R. damarensis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268138.t002

Prediction 1 and 2

Echolocation pulses of R. capensis and R. damarensis differed significantly in frequency, SL, and detection distances between species and biome (Generalised Linear Model (GLM): F = 239.35, DF = 5, 93, P = 0.00; Table 2).

Echolocation frequencies (kHz)

Within the desert biome, R. capensis emitted pulses of similar frequency during emergence and at the nearby foraging area (GLM: F = 64.54, DF = 5, 61, P = 0.00; unequal n HSD, P = 0.11). R. damarensis recorded emerging/foraging by the Orange River cave emitted pulses of slightly higher frequencies than those of R. damarensis recorded emerging from Numeesberg mine shaft (GLM: F = 13.61, DF = 5, 26, P = 0.00, unequal n HSD: P = 0.03) (Table 2). Comparing species, R. damarensis pulses were significantly higher in frequency than R. capensis pulses recorded in the desert (GLM: F = 293.35, DF = 5, 93, P = 0.00; unequal n HSD: P = 0.00).

Comparing biomes, echolocation pulses of R. capensis in the desert biome (open habitat) were significantly lower in frequency (unequal n HSD, P = 0.00) than R. capensis pulses recorded in the fynbos biome (cluttered habitat). R. damarensis echolocation pulse frequencies were not significantly different to the echolocation pulse frequencies recorded from R. capensis in the fynbos (unequal n HSD: P = 0.80).

Source levels (dB)

R. capensis recorded in the field had a wide dynamic range (maximum—minimum SLs) of 18 dB across individuals and biomes (Table 2). Within the desert biome R. capensis called louder in the foraging area than during emergence from Wondergat cave (GLM: F = 64.54, DF = 5, 61, P = 0.00; unequal n HSD, P = 0.00; Table 2). Comparing biomes (desert versus fynbos), average SLs for R. capensis in the desert were higher than those for R. capensis in the fynbos (unequal n HSD, P = 0.00). In addition, the maximum recorded SLs in the desert was higher than the maximum recorded SLs in the fynbos (Table 2).

The dynamic range of R. damarensis (28 dB) calculated over both sites was much wider than that in R. capensis (Table 2). SLs were not significantly different for R. damarensis between Numeesberg mine shaft and the Orange River cave (GLM: F = 13.61, DF = 5, 26, P = 0.00, unequal n HSD: P = 0.48; Table 2). On average R. damarensis emitted significantly lower SLs than R. capensis recorded in the desert (GLM: F = 293.35, DF = 5, 93, P = 0.00; unequal n HSD: P = 0.02) (as a consequence of lower minimum recorded SLs) but not significantly lower SLs than R. capensis in the fynbos (unequal n HSD: P = 0.16) biome (Table 2). However, maximum recorded SLs for R. damarensis (141 dB) were substantially higher than those recorded for R. capensis in both biomes (Table 2).

Prey detection distances (m)

Within the desert biome (during emergence versus at a nearby foraging area), there was no difference in detection distances for R. capensis between roost emergence and the foraging site for all prey sizes (GLM: F = 64.54, DF = 5, 61, P = 0.00; small, medium, and large: unequal n HSD, P > 0.05) (Table 2). Comparing biomes (desert versus fynbos), a combination of lower frequency and higher SL pulses yielded significantly longer detection distances (small, medium, and large: unequal n HSD, P < 0.05) for R. capensis in the desert than in the fynbos (Table 2).

Within the desert biome no significant differences in detection distances were found between R. damarensis recorded at Numeesberg mine shaft and the Orange River cave (GLM: F = 13.61, DF = 5,26, P = 0.00, small, medium, and large: unequal n HSD, P > 0.05). R. damarensis had lower average detection distances than R. capensis in the desert (GLM: F = 293.35, DF = 5, 93; P = 0.00 small, medium, large: unequal n HSD: p < 0.05) but not in the fynbos biome (unequal n HSD: p > 0.05) (Table 2). However, R. damarensis had higher maximum detection distances (5.3 m, 7.0 m, 8.2 m) (because of higher maximum SLs) than both desert (4.8 m, 6.6 m, 7.9 m) and fynbos R. capensis (4.1 m, 5.6 m, 6.8 m).

Relative contributions of frequency and SLs to detection distances (prediction 3)

Within biomes, SLs had a greater influence on detection distances of R. capensis than frequencies. In each habitat differences in prey detection distances calculated for R. capensis using both maximum and minimum (for that respective biome) SLs was greater than differences in prey detection distances calculated when using maximum and minimum recorded frequencies (Wilcoxon Matched Pairs Test: fynbos: Z = 4.62, P = 0.00, N = 28; desert: Z = 3.82, P = 0.00, N = 19) (Table 3). This effect (differences in prey detection distances when using either maximum or minimum recorded SLs) was larger for the fynbos biome than the desert (GLM: F = 3939, df = 15, 182, p = 0.00; unequal n HSD, p = 0.00) (Table 3). Differences in average recorded SLs between biomes (Table 4A) contributed to greater differences in prey detection within the desert biome than differences in recorded average frequencies (Table 4B; Friedman ANOVA: χ2 = 168, N = 21, P = 0.00). The greatest differences in detection distance were found when comparing detection distances calculated using both average frequencies and SLs of each biome (Table 4C; Friedman ANOVA: χ2 = 168, N = 21, P = 0.00).

thumbnail
Table 3. Differences (DIFF) (average ± SD) in calculated detection distances (DD) of small (S), medium (M) and large (L) prey of R. capensis (in both fynbos and desert biomes).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268138.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. A comparison of the differences in detection distances (average ± SD) of echolocation passes (n = 21 acoustic flight paths) when using different frequencies and SLs from the desert and fynbos biomes.

Detection distances were calculated using atmospheric conditions of flight paths recorded in the desert at Wondergat cave with a) average desert frequency with either average SL from the desert and fynbos biome b) average desert SL with average frequency from the desert and fynbos biome c) the average frequency and SL for each biome.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268138.t004

Discussion

The results of this study suggest that the use of low frequency echolocation pulses resulted in longer detection distances in R. capensis (as proposed by the FHH) but low frequencies may not be a prerequisite for successful foraging in open biomes. R. capensis in the desert had longer detection distances than R. capensis in the fynbos (Prediction 1) because of both lower frequency and higher source level pulses. Lower frequency, higher SL pulses travel further than higher frequency, lower SL pulses, probably allowing R. capensis in the desert to search greater volumes of open space in the desert biome more efficiently. Higher SLs used by R. capensis in the desert had a greater contribution to observed differences in detection distances between biomes than frequency (Prediction 3). In addition, on average, R. damarensis did not compensate for higher frequencies with higher SLs (Prediction 2) resulting in shorter average detection distances than R. capensis in the desert but not the fynbos (Prediction 1). These results suggest that both predictions were not supported and that the lower frequencies used by R. capensis in the desert may not be a consequence of selection for longer detection distances. However, a few measurements of SLs for R. damarensis were the highest recorded and resulted in the longest prey detection distances recorded in this study. The rare use of such high SLs by R. damarensis in this study, despite the resultant increase in detection distances, may be a consequence of the energetic costs associated with higher SLs [68]. Nevertheless, a combination of both SLs and frequencies produced the greatest differences in detection distances in R. capensis between biomes. Therefore, the possibility that lower frequency pulses were, at least in part, selected because they allowed longer detection distance in R. capensis in the desert cannot be excluded.

According to our modified version of the Foraging Habitat Hypothesis (FHH) the proposed ecological significance of lower frequency echolocation pulses in clutter forager specialists (HDC bats) in open habitats (desert) is that lower frequencies increase detection distances [10]. In this study, lower frequency echolocation pulses in R. capensis in the desert resulted in longer detection distances than R. capensis in the fynbos. However, R. damarensis, the heterospecific species that inhabits the same desert habitat as R. capensis but uses a higher pulse frequency, had shorter average detection distances to R. capensis in the desert (but not fynbos). Contrary to Prediction 2 they did not compensate for these lower pulse frequencies with higher average SLs. This is contrary to the findings of Surlykke and Kalko [30] that bats within local assemblages compensated for frequency-dependent losses in sound propagation by using higher SLs to achieve comparable prey detection distances. An important caveat to our results were the few high SLs found in R. damarensis, recorded outside both cave entrances, that produced longer detection distances than those of both fynbos–and desert–inhabiting R. capensis. These few data points from R. damarensis might alternatively provide support for Surlykke and Kalko [30] findings as well as the FHH but more research is required to definitively test this, and the effect that energetic costs have on echolocation SLs of bats. In addition to frequency and SL, beam width has also been shown to be a contributing factor to sonar distance [58,59]. In this study we were unable to measure beam width with our microphone array design.

Recent findings by Currie et al. [68] have shown that above 130 dB SPL the metabolic cost of sound production is extremely high. The recorded SLs for both R. capensis and R. damarensis fell in the range emitted by free-ranging LDC echolocators (120 dB—140 dB) [29,30,48] despite marked differences in echolocation pulse frequency between the different species. Our results and those from other studies suggest that SLs have a large impact on detection distances of bats [30,68]. Currie et al. [68] showed that for every dB increase in SL beyond 120 dB SPL Pipistrellus nathusii could detect an insect an extra 15 cm away. In our study, SL contributed more to differences in detection distance between biomes than frequency. In addition, when R. damarensis did emit higher SLs they were able to achieve longer detection distances than desert-inhabiting R. capensis despite having higher frequencies. However, the benefits of increased SLs (i.e. longer detection distances) are sometimes outweighed by the costs. Refraining from emitting high SLs when the costs of such high emissions does not justify the benefits (such as in high clutter or where prey capture is unlikely) could optimize energetic costs. This is evident in the lower SLs recorded in laboratory versus field environments [30,3335,48,69] as well as during emergence versus foraging, when prey capture is more likely (our study). It is also possible that these energetic constraints limit bats from constantly emitting pulses with such high SLs. High energy costs could explain the higher maximum, but not average SLs recorded in this study for R. damarensis compared to desert-inhabiting R. capensis.

In R. capensis the evolution of lower frequency pulses combined with the use of higher SLs may allow detection distances to be maximized in the open desert while keeping energy costs low. Mutavhatsindi [32] found similar findings in two species of LDC bats with different foraging strategies and habitats (open air versus clutter). Jacobs et al. [70] showed that the frequencies used by R. capensis at De Hoop are audible to common moths [32,70]. Therefore, in addition to the conservation of energy, using lower SL pulses could minimize the chances of being detected by prey. However, if a combination of SL and frequency is optimal in desert bats then why has R. damarensis not followed the same evolutionary trajectory as R. capensis (i.e. evolution of lower frequency pulses). The evolution of lower frequency pulses in R. capensis in the desert may have therefore evolved for additional purposes other than an increase in detection distances.

The co-inhabitance of two similar species (R. capensis and R. damarensis) occupying the same desert environment but using different echolocation pulse frequencies suggests an alternative explanation of why R. capensis in the desert uses frequencies below 82 kHz. A frequency which deviates more than any other population of R. capensis from the allometric relationship between body size and pulse frequency for this species [27]. The Acoustic Communication Hypothesis (ACH) [27,40,71] proposes that R. capensis uses lower frequencies to prevent acoustic overlap with R. damarensis maintaining effective intraspecific communication. In habituation-dishabituation experiments Bastian and Jacobs [40] found that R. capensis were able to discriminate between their own pulses and those of R. damarensis. However, it is important to note that the closest roosts (Wondergat Cave and Numeesberg Mine Shaft) of these species in the desert are 15.8 km apart and their foraging areas might not overlap. Whether they are syntonic, as required by the ACH, is therefore unknown. If the ACH is valid (i.e. differences in frequency are required for species discrimination) then lower frequency pulses in the desert would not only offer longer detection distance, without excess energy expenditure, but also enable unambiguous intra–specific communication.

Frequency has also been shown to be related to prey composition as proposed by the Prey Detection Hypothesis [63,7274]. In R. capensis differences in frequency between biomes are not likely due to differences in dietary composition. Insects sampled in this study fell within the same size range giving them equal target strengths [9,72] in both biomes making it unlikely that there is selection pressure from different sized insects. The Sensory Drive Hypothesis (SDH) proposes that climate-induced differences in atmospheric attenuation selects for lower frequency echolocation pulses [75]. The SDH hypothesis was used to explain why Rhinolophus bats inhabiting dry habitats produce pulses with higher frequencies than those found occupying humid habitats [12,19,71]. Some studies in HDC bats have demonstrated a complex relationship between frequency and other climatic variables (e.g. atmospheric pressure, temperature, altitude) [12,19,76]. For example, in arid but not mesic habitats [12,19] temperature was found to have the overriding influence on atmospheric attenuation which could explain why in R. capensis no correlation between humidity and frequency was found between biomes [24]. The pulse frequencies and detection distances used by R. capensis in both desert and fynbos biomes in this study contradict the SDH. According to this hypothesis, pulses with lower frequencies and therefore longer detection distances should occur in the fynbos environment because it experiences hotter, more humid average nightly conditions (13.90°C, 84%) than the desert (12.05°C, 73%) (South African Weather Service). The SDH hypothesis may explain variation in pulse frequencies used by other rhinolophid species [12,19,76] but it does not appear to explain variation in the observed pulse frequencies and SLs of R. capensis.

In conclusion, in small HDC bats lower frequency pulses do offer the conferred advantage of longer detection distances without having to exert excess energy expenditure into calling above 130 dB (a decibel range that is associated with a significant increase in energy expenditure in small bats) [68]. However these lower frequencies might not be a prerequisite for successful foraging in open environments suggesting that lower frequency pulses could have evolved in R. capensis in the desert for additional purposes (such as intraspecific communication). Our study provides the first measurements of SLs and detection distances used by HDC bats in different biomes in the field. These measurements proved vital to improving our understanding of the ecological significance of frequency differences of clutter forager specialist living in different environments.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to David Wechuli and Itani Victor Mutavhatsindi for their help in the field and for the insect size analysis. We thank Megan Mehnert, Aurora Christie, Sydney Moyo, and Jake Sayles for their tireless efforts helping collect field data. We would also like to thank the following colleagues for their esteemed input: Anna Bastian, Robert Barclay, Stewart McCulloch and Tinyiko Maluleke. We are incredibly grateful to Neil Bredekamp, Ullrich Mutzeck and Derick September for constructing the arrays. We appreciate the reviewers for donating their time and knowledge to reading and critiquing this manuscript. We acknowledge the NRF (GUN 64798) and UCT for providing funding to David Jacobs and Nikita Finger.

References

  1. 1. Griffin DR. Listening in the dark: The acoustic orientation of bats and men. Yale Univer. Press; 1958.
  2. 2. Neuweiler G. How bats detect flying insects. Phys Today. 1980;33: 34–40.
  3. 3. Jung K, Kalko EKV, Helversen OV. Echolocation calls in Central American emballonurid bats: signal design and call frequency alternation. J Zool. 2007;272: 125–137.
  4. 4. Griffin DR, Webster FA, Michael CR. The echolocation of flying insects by bats. Anim Behav. 1960;8: 141–154.
  5. 5. Kingston T, Jones G, Akbar Z, Kunz TH. Echolocation signal design in Kerivoulinae and Murininae (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) from Malaysia. J Zool. 1999;249: 359–374.
  6. 6. Jung K, Molinari J, Kalko EKV. Driving factors for the evolution of species-specific echolocation call design in new world free-tailed bats (Molossidae). PLoS One. 2014;9: e85279. pmid:24454833
  7. 7. Wund MA. Learning and the development of habitat-specific bat echolocation. Anim Behav. 2005;70: 441–450.
  8. 8. Siemers BM, Schnitzler H-U. Echolocation signals reflect niche differentiation in five sympatric congeneric bat species. Nature. 2004;429: 657–661. pmid:15190352
  9. 9. Stilz W-P, Schnitzler H-U. Estimation of the acoustic range of bat echolocation for extended targets. J Acoust Soc Am. 2012;132: 1765–75. pmid:22978903
  10. 10. Jones G, Barlow KE. Cryptic species of echolocating bats. Echolocation in bats and dolphins. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago; 2004. pp. 345–349.
  11. 11. Luo J, Koselj K, Zsebok S, Siemers BM, Goerlitz HR. Global warming alters sound transmission: differential impact on the prey detection ability of echolocating bats. J Royal Soc Interface Royal Soc. 2013;11: 20130961. pmid:24335559
  12. 12. Mutumi GL, Jacobs DS, Winker H. Sensory drive mediated by climatic gradients partially explains divergence in acoustic signals in two horseshoe bat species, Rhinolophus swinnyi and Rhinolophus simulator. PLoS One. 2016;11: e0148053. pmid:26815436
  13. 13. Barclay RM, Fullard JH, Jacobs DS. Variation in the echolocation calls of the hoary bat (Lasiurus cinereus): influence of body size, habitat structure, and geographic location. Can J Zool. 1999;77: 530–534.
  14. 14. Snell-Rood EC. The effect of climate on acoustic signals: Does atmospheric sound absorption matter for bird song and bat echolocation? J Acoust Soc Am. 2012;131: 1650–1658. pmid:22352535
  15. 15. Suga N, O’Neill WE, Kujirai K, Manabe T. Specificity of combination-sensitive neurons for processing of complex biosonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J Neurophysiol. 1983;49: 1573–1626. pmid:6875639
  16. 16. Neuweiler G. Foraging ecology and audition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evol. 1989;4: 160–166. pmid:21227342
  17. 17. Schuller G, Pollak G. Disproportionate frequency representation in the inferior colliculus of doppler-compensating Greater Horseshoe bats: Evidence for an acoustic fovea. J Comp Physiology. 1979;132: 47–54.
  18. 18. Xu Z, Jing W, Keping S, Tinglei J, Yunlei J, Jiang F. Echolocation calls of Rhinolophus ferrumequinum in relation to habitat type and environmental factors. Acta Ecol Sinica. 2008;28: 5248–5258.
  19. 19. Jacobs DS, Catto S, Mutumi GL, Finger N, Webala PW. Testing the sensory drive hypothesis: geographic variation in echolocation frequencies of Geoffroy’s horseshoe bat (Rhinolophidae: Rhinolophus clivosus). PLoS One. 2017;12: e0187769. pmid:29186147
  20. 20. Maluleke T, Jacobs DS, Winker H. Environmental correlates of geographic divergence in a phenotypic trait: A case study using bat echolocation. Ecol Evol. 2017;7: 7347–7361. pmid:28944021
  21. 21. Neuweiler G. Foraging, echolocation and audition in bats. Naturwissenschaften. 1984;71: 446–455.
  22. 22. Norberg UM, Rayner JM. Ecological morphology and flight in bats (Mammalia; Chiroptera): wing adaptations, flight performance, foraging strategy and echolocation. Philosophical Transactions Royal Soc Lond B Biological Sci. 1987;316: 335–427.
  23. 23. Siemers B, Kalko E, Schnitzler H-U. Echolocation behavior and signal plasticity in the Neotropical bat Myotis nigricans (Vespertilionidae): a convergent case with European species of Pipistrellus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2001;50: 317–328.
  24. 24. Odendaal LJ, Jacobs DS, Bishop JM. Sensory trait variation in an echolocating bat suggests roles for both selection and plasticity. BMC Evol Biol. 2014;14: 60. pmid:24674227
  25. 25. Fenton Faure, Ratcliffe. Evolution of high duty cycle echolocation in bats. J Exp Biol. 2012;215: 2935–2944. pmid:22875762
  26. 26. Pye JD, Roberts LH. Ear Movements in a Hipposiderid Bat. Nature. 1970;225: 285–286.
  27. 27. Jacobs DS, Barclay RM, Walker MH. The allometry of echolocation call frequencies of insectivorous bats: why do some species deviate from the pattern? Oecologia. 2007;152: 583–94. pmid:17345101
  28. 28. Holderied MW, Korine C, Fenton MB, Parsons S, Robson S, Jones G. Echolocation call intensity in the aerial hawking bat Eptesicus bottae (Vespertilionidae) studied using stereo videogrammetry. J Exp Biol. 2005;208: 1321–1327. pmid:15781892
  29. 29. Jensen ME, Miller LA. Echolocation signals of the bat Eptesicus serotinus recorded using a vertical microphone array: effect of flight altitude on searching signals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1999;47: 60–69.
  30. 30. Surlykke A, Kalko EK. Echolocating bats cry out loud to detect their prey. PLoS One. 2008;3: e2036. pmid:18446226
  31. 31. Surlykke A, Miller LA, Møhl B, Andersen BB, Christensen-Dalsgaard J, Jørgensen MB. Echolocation in two very small bats from Thailand Craseonycteris thonglongyai and Myotis siligorensis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1993;33: 1–12.
  32. 32. Mutavhatsindi IV. The influence of foraging habitat on acoustic signal source levels in two bat species, Neoromicia capensis (Vespertilionidae) and Tadarida aegyptiaca (Molossidae). 2018.
  33. 33. Schnitzler H-U, Grinnell AD. Directional sensitivity of echolocation in the horseshoe bat, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum: Directionality of sound emission. J Comp Physiology. 1977;116: 51–61.
  34. 34. Schuchmann M, Siemers BM. Variability in echolocation call intensity in a community of horseshoe bats: a role for resource partitioning or communication? PLoS One. 2010;5. pmid:20862252
  35. 35. Waters DA, Jones G. Echolocation call structure and intensity in five species of insectivorous bats. J Exp Biology. 1995;198: 475–89. pmid:7699316
  36. 36. Jacobs D, Bastian A. High duty cycle echolocation may constrain the evolution of diversity within horseshoe bats (family: Rhinolophidae). Diversity. 2018;10: 85.
  37. 37. Brinkløv S, Jakobsen L, Ratcliffe JM, Kalko EKV, Surlykke A. Echolocation call intensity and directionality in flying short-tailed fruit bats, Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae). J Acoust Soc Am. 2011;129: 427–35. pmid:21303022
  38. 38. Jakobsen L, Brinkløv S, Surlykke A. Intensity and directionality of bat echolocation signals. Front Physiol. 2013;4: 89. pmid:23630501
  39. 39. Csorba G, Peter U, Thomas N. Horseshoe bats of the world:(Chiroptera: Rhinolophidae). Alana Books; 2003.
  40. 40. Bastian A, Jacobs DS. Listening carefully: increased perceptual acuity for species discrimination in multispecies signalling assemblages. Anim Behav. 2015;101: 141–154.
  41. 41. Dool SE, Puechmaille SJ, Foley NM, Allegrini B, Bastian A, Mutumi GL, et al. Nuclear introns outperform mitochondrial DNA in inter-specific phylogenetic reconstruction: Lessons from horseshoe bats (Rhinolophidae: Chiroptera). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2016;97: 196–212. pmid:26826601
  42. 42. Mucina L, Rutherford MC. The vegetation of South Africa, Lesotho and Swaziland. South African National Biodiversity Institute, Pretoria. 2006.
  43. 43. Matthews T, Measey GJ, Roberts DL. Implications of summer breeding frogs from Langebaanweg, South Africa: Regional climate evolution at 5.1 mya. S Afr J Sci. 2016;112.
  44. 44. Bamba A, Diallo I, Touré NE, Kouadio K, Konaré A, Dramé MS, et al. Effect of the African greenbelt position on West African summer climate: a regional climate modeling study. Theor Appl Climatol. 2018;137: 309–322.
  45. 45. Turner W, Spector S, Gardiner N, Fladeland M, Sterling E, Steininger M. Remote sensing for biodiversity science and conservation. Trends Ecol Evol. 2003;18: 306–314.
  46. 46. Brandstein MS, Adcock JE, Silverman HF. A practical time-delay estimator for localizing speech sources with a microphone array. Comput Speech Lang. 1995;9: 153–169.
  47. 47. Gillette MD, Silverman HF. A linear closed-form algorithm for source localization from time-differences of arrival. Ieee Signal Proc Let. 2008;15: 1–4.
  48. 48. Holderied M, Helversen O von. Echolocation range and wingbeat period match in aerial-hawking bats. Proc R Soc Lond. 2003. pmid:14613617
  49. 49. Gudra T, Furmankiewicz J, Herman K. Bats sonar calls and its application in sonar systems. In: Kolev N, editor. Sonar Systems. Rijeka Croatia: InTech; 2011. pp. 209–234.
  50. 50. Aubauer R. Dreidimensionale flugbahnverfolgung von flederma¨usen fortschritte der akustik. DAGA 94. 1994.
  51. 51. Aubauer R, Ruppert C. Untersuchung von Mikrofonanordnungen zur passiven Ortung von Schallquellen Fortschritte der Akustik. DAGA 94. 1994.
  52. 52. Holderied MW, Jones G, Helversen O von. Flight and echolocation behaviour of whiskered bats commuting along a hedgerow: range-dependent sonar signal design, Doppler tolerance and evidence for “acoustic focussing”. J Exp Biology. 2006;209: 1816–26. pmid:16651548
  53. 53. Simmons JA, Stein RA. Acoustic imaging in bat sonar: Echolocation signals and the evolution of echolocation. J Comp Physiology. 1980;135: 61–84.
  54. 54. Simmons JA. Echolocation: resolution of target range by bats. J Acoust Soc Am. 1967;42: 1188–1188.
  55. 55. Simmons JA. The resolution of target range by echolocating bats. J Acoust Soc Am. 1973;54: 157–173. pmid:4738624
  56. 56. Ey E, Fischer J. The “Acoustic Adaptation Hypothesis”—A review of the evidence from birds, anurans, and mammals. Bioacoustics. 2009;19: 21–48.
  57. 57. Sternheim MMS, Kane JW. General physics. Sons JW and, editor. New York, NY: Wiley; 1986.
  58. 58. Jakobsen L, Ratcliffe JM, Surlykke A. Convergent acoustic field of view in echolocating bats. Nature. 2013;493: 93–6. pmid:23172147
  59. 59. Surlykke A, Pedersen SB, Jakobsen L. Echolocating bats emit a highly directional sonar sound beam in the field. Proc R Soc Lond. 2009;276: 853–60. pmid:19129126
  60. 60. Møhl B. Target detection by echolocating bats. Animal Sonar. Boston, MA: Springer; 1988. pp. 435–450. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-7493-0_43
  61. 61. Goerlitz HR, Hofstede HM ter, Zeale MRK, Jones G, Holderied MW. An aerial-hawking bat uses stealth echolocation to counter moth hearing. Curr Biol. 2010;20: 1568–1572. pmid:20727755
  62. 62. Bazley EN. Sound absorption in air at frequencies up to 100 kHz. In: (England) NPLT, editor. National Physical Laboratory (Great Britain). 1976.
  63. 63. Waters DA, Rydell J, Jones G. Echolocation call design and limits on prey size: a case study using the aerial-hawking bat Nyctalus leisleri. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1995;37: 321–328.
  64. 64. Grimbacher PS, Stork NE. Vertical stratification of feeding guilds and body size in beetle assemblages from an Australian tropical rainforest. Austral Ecol. 2007;32: 77–85.
  65. 65. Morse DR, Stork NE, Lawton JH. Species number, species abundance and body length relationships of arboreal beetles in Bornean lowland rain forest trees. Ecol Entomol. 1988;13: 25–37.
  66. 66. Sikes RS, Mammalogists AC and UC of the AS of. 2016 Guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of wild mammals in research and education. J Mammal. 2016;97: 663–688. pmid:29692469
  67. 67. Committee AC and U. Guidelines for the capture, handling, and care of mammals as Approved by the American Society of Mammalogists. J Mammal. 1998;79: 1416–1431.
  68. 68. Currie SE, Boonman A, Troxell S, Yovel Y, Voigt CC. Echolocation at high intensity imposes metabolic costs on flying bats. Nat Ecol Evol. 2020; 1–4. pmid:32661405
  69. 69. Boonman A, Jones G. Intensity control during target approach in echolocating bats; stereotypical sensori-motor behaviour in Daubenton’s bats, Myotis daubentonii. J Exp Biology. 2002;205: 2865–74.
  70. 70. Jacobs DS, Ratcliffe JM, Fullard JH. Beware of bats, beware of birds: the auditory responses of eared moths to bat and bird predation. Behav Ecol. 2008;19: 1333–1342.
  71. 71. Heller K-G, Helversen OV. Resource partitioning of sonar frequency bands in rhinolophoid bats. Oecologia. 1989;80: 178–186. pmid:28313104
  72. 72. Houston RD, Boonman AM, Jones G. Do echolocation signal parameters restrict bats’ choice of prey? Echolocation in bats and dolphins. 2004. p. 345.
  73. 73. Barclay RMR. The echolocation calls of hoary (Lasiurus cinereus) and silver-haired (Lasionycteris noctivagans) bats as adaptations for long- versus short-range foraging strategies and the consequences for prey selection. Can J Zool. 1986;64: 2700–2705.
  74. 74. Brigham RM. Prey detection, dietary niche breadth, and body size in bats: why are aerial insectivorous bats so small? Am Nat. 1991;137: 693–703.
  75. 75. Endler JA. Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am Nat. 1992;139: S125–S153.
  76. 76. Jiang T, Liu R, Metzner W, You Y, Li S, Liu S, et al. Geographical and individual variation in echolocation calls of the intermediate leaf‐nosed bat, Hipposideros larvatus. Ethology. 2010;116: 691–703.